1. Introduction
Scientists have found that several parts of the human brain can share the same cognitive functions. For example, the experimenters have found that emotional and valuative processing overlap in the areas of the parietal, motor, and sensory cortex, which also process mathematics [1]. Recent findings found that music and speech cognitive functions have many aspects in common and that some neural modules are shared for speech and music [2]. For instance, musicians display stronger activations in the brain area related to language function than non-musicians, which implies using the same neural networks for music and language functions [3]. More detailed studies were done to test the hypothesis that music and language functions may share some of the same brain parts. For instance, the processing need of musical syntax (harmony) and language syntax interact in Broca’s area in the left inferior frontal gyrus (without leading to music and language main effects) [4].
Besides, speech functions can benefit from music functions, which further proves the linkage between music and language: brief exposure to a Mozart sonata promotes short-term increases in spatial-reasoning abilities [5]; pre-schoolers can benefit from a program of musical training to enhance their phonological awareness [6]. Much more hypotheses and tests to prove them have been done than what is listed here.
However, there is seldom research on the emotional links between language and music. Also, most research focused on the influence from several perspectives of music to the perspectives of language. An experiment was conducted to directly test how language influences cognition toward music from an emotional aspect; specifically, how the lyrics affect the emotional perception of a melody will be tested.
2. Hypothesis
The main target of the proposed study is to figure out whether language affects the emotional perception toward music, more specifically, whether the emotions people perceive from melody are influenced by their familiarity with language; thus, for the hypothesis of the proposed study, if participants are more familiar with the language used in the lyrics, they will perceive stronger feeling from music. On the contrary, if the test does not support the previous hypothesis, whether participants are more familiar with the language used in the lyrics, they will perceive the same feeling from music. If the hypothesis can be proved by the test, then the participant's familiarity with the language used in the lyrics was directly proportional to how they felt.
2.1. Participants
One hundred participants were selected randomly between 18 and 40 years old from Mandarin monolinguals and English monolinguals, no bilinguals.
2.2. Materials
Two sets of melodies, one composed by a Chinese composer and one composed by a US composer, should express happy feelings. 2 sets of lyrics for the melodies, one in Mandarin happy expression and one in English.
2.3. Procedure
Each subject hears each melody presented once, with either Mandarin or English, and either a happy or sad set of lyrics; whether a given melody is presented with happy or sad lyrics and with Mandarin or English lyrics will be counterbalanced across cultures. Each subject will hear 4 melodies, 2 of which are happy, 2 of which are in their native language. They will be divided into groups to do the 2 (Native language (Mandarin, English)) x 2(Country of the composer (Chinese, US)) x 2 (Language of lyrics (Mandarin, English)) x 1 (Valence of melody (Happy)) design. Each group will be required to hear one of that music. Every time participants hear the melody. They will be required to choose from the following options that can describe the feelings they think the people in the video are feeling: 1 represents very sad, 2 represents sad, 3 represents neutral, 4 represents happy, and 5 represents very happy. This experiment will only test people's reactions to positive melodies, so it can focus more on measuring the emotion rating rather than the impact on negative and positive. After that, the differences in positive emotion ratings as a function of whether each melody is presented with happy or sad lyrics and whether the speakers know the language of the lyrics or not could be tested.
3. Prediction
This experiment predicts that emotions people perceive from music are influenced by their familiarity with the language. The more familiar participants are with the language used in the lyrics, the stronger the feelings they will perceive. The experiment has 16 experimental groups, as each composer will compose two melodies that they consider positive. Chinese melodies are CHM1 and CHM2, and U.S. melodies are USM1 and USM2. Moreover, each melody is accompanied by four types of lyrics, including both positive and negative Chinese lyrics and both positive and negative U.S. lyrics.
When native melodies are accompanied by native language lyrics, the prediction is that positive lyrics will bring a higher positive emotion rating, as shown in Figure 1a, and negative lyrics will bring a lower positive emotion rating, as shown in Figure 1b. Differently, when melodies accompany foreign language lyrics, positive and negative lyrics may bring similar outcomes, as participants cannot understand foreign languages.
It is also possible that even if participants don’t understand the lyrics, they might perceive a positive emotion. For example, for those who receive native melodies and positive or negative foreign lyrics, the positive emotion rating may still be at a relatively high level, as shown in Figure 2a. Differently, for those who receive foreign melodies and foreign positive or negative lyrics, participants might gain positive emotion, but the rating is much lower, as shown in Figure 2b.
Note: Predictions of the proposed study (y-axis represents percentage of each rating)
Figure 1:(a)Native melodies and native positive lyrics, a higher positive emotion rating.(b)Native melodies and native negative lyrics, a lower positive emotion rating.
Figure 2:(a)Native melodies and foreign lyrics still bring a relatively high positive emotion rating.(b)Foreign melodies and lyrics bring a lower positive emotion rating.
4. Conclusion
If the results show that people obtain higher positive or negative emotion ratings while listening to native melodies accompanied by native language lyrics than those accompanied by foreign language lyrics, it means that emotions people perceive from music are influenced by their familiarity with the language.
Alternatively, if the results show that people obtain similar positive or negative emotion ratings while listening to native melodies accompanied by native language compared to those accompanied with foreign language lyrics, it means that people’s emotions perceived from music are not be influenced by their familiarity with the language.
On the other hand, if the results support that people obtain higher positive or negative emotion ratings while listening to native melodies accompanied by a foreign language than foreign melodies accompanied by a foreign language, it means that emotions people perceive from music are influenced by their familiarity with melody.
The result of this study could help contribute to a better role for music in psychotherapy or even integrate music into psychotherapy. From this study, it can also be inferred what type of music people are most likely to be influenced by, and thus, psychotherapists can improve the treatment of specific ailments through music. In addition, by unveiling what kind of songs the public likes, this study can help composers make better-selling tunes catering to the public appetite, as their understanding of the public preference has a big impact on how well a song sells.
One imperfection of this study is that it is difficult to distinguish whether it is language or culture that truly influenced people’s emotions perceived from music. People from different countries were chosen to participate in the experiment to make sure their native language is different, those from different countries largely have different cultures. The understanding of emotions and music may vary in different cultures, potentially influencing the experimental results. Further study can focus on whether culture influences the emotion that people perceive from music.
To add to that, more generalizable tests are needed. This experiment only tests differences between Western and Eastern cultures, specifically USA and Chinese cultures, to avoid possible obvious confounding variables across cultures. It is unclear what the result would be in other cultures, such as the ones from Japan, Korea, and Indonesia from Eastern countries, and Russia, Britain, and France from Western countries.
Besides, some confounding variables are hard to avoid. For example, people may have different musical familiarity - some may feel more sympathetic toward the same melody than others; therefore, the more sympathetic participants may rate the quiz with more extreme scores. It is hard to know if this incident affects the score. Moreover, testing a participant’s sensitivity to be sympathetic when listening to a melody is expensive because experimenters may have to use expensive technological devices to detect the activity in the brain, such as EEG or fMRI. Future researchers may include this test if they follow the proposal of this work.
Acknowledgment
All authors contributed equally to this work and should be considered co-first authors.
References
[1]. Hsu, C.-W., & Goh, J. O. (2016). Distinct and overlapping brain areas engaged during value-based, mathematical, and emotional decision processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00275
[2]. Tallal, P., and Gaab, N. (2006). Dynamic auditory processing, musical experience and language development. Trends Neurosci. 29, 382–390.
[3]. Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., Rotte, M., Drescher, D., Hinrichs, H., ... &Altenmüller, E. (2006). Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: evidence from fMRI conjunction. Neuroimage, 30(3), 917-926.
[4]. Kunert R, Willems RM, Casasanto D, Patel AD, Hagoort P (2015) Music and Language Syntax Interact in Broca’s Area: An fMRI Study. PLoS ONE 10( 11): e0141069. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0141069
[5]. Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, C. N. (1993). Music and spatial task performance. Nature, 365(6447), 611-611.
[6]. Dege, F., and Schwarzer, G. (2011). The effect of a music program on phonological awareness in preschoolers. Front. Psychol. 2:124. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00124
Cite this article
Liang,Y.;Lin,Z. (2024). The Influence of the Language Used in the Lyrics on How People Perceive the Emotions in Music. Lecture Notes in Education Psychology and Public Media,49,52-55.
Data availability
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study will be available from the authors upon reasonable request.
Disclaimer/Publisher's Note
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of EWA Publishing and/or the editor(s). EWA Publishing and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
About volume
Volume title: Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Interdisciplinary Humanities and Communication Studies
© 2024 by the author(s). Licensee EWA Publishing, Oxford, UK. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license. Authors who
publish this series agree to the following terms:
1. Authors retain copyright and grant the series right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgment of the work's authorship and initial publication in this
series.
2. Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the series's published
version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgment of its initial
publication in this series.
3. Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and
during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See
Open access policy for details).
References
[1]. Hsu, C.-W., & Goh, J. O. (2016). Distinct and overlapping brain areas engaged during value-based, mathematical, and emotional decision processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00275
[2]. Tallal, P., and Gaab, N. (2006). Dynamic auditory processing, musical experience and language development. Trends Neurosci. 29, 382–390.
[3]. Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., Rotte, M., Drescher, D., Hinrichs, H., ... &Altenmüller, E. (2006). Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: evidence from fMRI conjunction. Neuroimage, 30(3), 917-926.
[4]. Kunert R, Willems RM, Casasanto D, Patel AD, Hagoort P (2015) Music and Language Syntax Interact in Broca’s Area: An fMRI Study. PLoS ONE 10( 11): e0141069. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0141069
[5]. Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, C. N. (1993). Music and spatial task performance. Nature, 365(6447), 611-611.
[6]. Dege, F., and Schwarzer, G. (2011). The effect of a music program on phonological awareness in preschoolers. Front. Psychol. 2:124. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00124