1. Introduction
Since it was proposed by Thorndike in 1920, the halo effect has been a significant cognitive bias in the field of psychology. In the 20th century, researchers mostly focused on the definitions, classifications, and measurement of halo effect [1]. While in the 21st century, with behavioral economics thriving as a new field of inquiry, the potential of halo effect as a heuristic has been accentuated. Existing literature reveals that halo effect is a notable heuristic in affecting people’s decision making processes in many items relevant to corporate business and management. Thus, it is significant to review literature on the halo effect and conclude its applications at the corporate level. This essay reviews the literature written by international scholars from 1920 to 2022 on the definition, classification and measurement of the halo effect, and examines its applications in both the business and management levels for corporations, focusing on brand marketing and HRM practices respectively. This essay also concludes the unsolved problems of halo effect literature and identifies the directions for future scholars. This essay sorts the existing literature to provide scholars with more specific research focusing on this significant effect.
2. Definition of the Halo Effect
Scholars have long been bewildered by the inconsistency in the definitions of halo effect. When Thorndike studies the performance rating for flight commanders, he finds that the ratings for physique, leadership and character are all higher than reality [1]. He concludes that the general merit of an employee could be extended to influence the ratings for special abilities, or vice versa. Therefore, he defines such effect as a marked tendency to think of the person in general as rather good or rather inferior and to color the judgments of the specific performance dimensions by this general feeling. As the original definition of halo effect, it accentuates halo effect as the rater’s property, which is supported or challenged in future studies.
In the following decades, different scholars have given their understandings and definitions of the halo effect. For instance, Johnson claims in the world’s first published verification of the halo effect that when raters rate several individuals on several traits, they will rate some individuals high on most traits and some low on most traits [2]. If the ratings of the individuals on any two traits are correlated, the correlation will be positive and often quite large. He also proves with experimental results that halo effect is due to an error of judgement rather than to objective facts [2][3]. Subsequently, Saal et al. define the halo effect as a tendency to attend to a global impression of each ratee rather than to carefully distinguish among levels of different performance dimensions, or a rater's inability or unwillingness to distinguish among the dimensions of a given ratee’s job behavior [4].
The inconsistency of halo effect’s definition derives from its ambiguous substance. According to Thorndike’s original definition of the halo effect, it is a property of the rater, meaning that some raters are relatively more inclined to commit this error, which is supported by many scholars [5][6][7][8][9][10]. However, as Symonds suggests, the effect resides in the judging process, but it might not be the rater’s characteristic [11]. According to Kaplan, the halo effect attributes rating changes to an amalgamation of trait and overall person valuations [12]. Later, Murphy proves with a quantitative research that halo effect is actually a characteristic of the unique rating situation [13]. Until today, Murphy’s perspective about the substance of halo was the most accepted idea, but controversy still remains.
3. Classification of Halo
With the controversy over the definitions of halo effect coming to a deadlock, scholars realize the significance of distinguishing the halo effect into different categories, which could help them understand halo from different directions.
To eliminate the confusion surrounding the conceptual definition of halo, Balzer and Sulsky defines general impression halo and dimensional similarity halo [14]. While the former is defined as a general impression bias, the latter is defined as a logical error, both of which derive from previous studies on the halo effect [13]. This classification is concluded by reviewing previous literature, but it lacks practical value for further investigation. Mathematically, scholars have distinguished two types of halo: covariance between dimension scores and co-occurrence of scoring levels on different dimensions [15][16][17]. This classification method has proven to be important for scholars who try to measure halo.
Another influential classification is Bingham’s concepts of valid halo(true halo) and invalid halo(illusory halo), which has become the most influential classification [18]. While the true halo is the expected overlap among traits and the overlap between each trait and an over-all or summary judgment, the illusory halo refers to any excess overlap in these situations beyond what might be expected. This study highlights the idea that the emergence of halo might indicate inaccuracy. On the contrary, it is possible to show the real relationship among different traits. While Bingham’s classification has influenced later studies, scholars have still been struggling to distinguish true halo and illusory halo, despite the fact that some new methods have been proposed, but lacking further examination [19]. Future studies should examine the new methods to distinguish true and illusory halo, which can add much enrichment to the halo effect literature.
4. Applications of Halo Effect at the Corporate Level
Existing literature has proven that the halo effect is highly influential in corporations, especially in brand marketing and HRM practices.
4.1. Halo in Corporate Business
As Berry and Parasuraman claim, the purpose of brand marketing is to intensify strong relationships with loyal customers and to transform indifferent customers into loyal ones [20]. As is proven, the halo effect allows people to believe a subject is overall good or bad, the same way consumers could be convinced that an enterprise is overall excellent [21][22].
To distinguish halo from consumer behaviors, multiattribute attitude models are developed [23][24][25][26]. Multiattribute attitude models are a series of models that could distinguish various factors contributing to consumers’ attitudes and beliefs on certain brands or products. Based on these models, brand equity effect is explained by halo effect, meaning that the reason why brands could increase products’ prices is that customers are influenced by the halo [26][27][28].
With more aspects of brand marketing being studied, more practical conclusions have been drawn. For instance, with the Internet thriving, the competition between traditional supermarkets and online stores has become notable. While some executives are concerned that online stores would alleviate a brand’s value, researchers confirm that brand names become less valuable only when more total information about product attributes is available online, since comprehensive information urges consumers to be more aware of the traits of the products instead of the brand names [28]. However, consumers’ perceptions of the retailer’s online attributes and attitudes are indeed influenced by the image of the prior offline brand [29].
Another trend in the 21st century is globalization. The flourishing international trade has shown that country of origin information could alter consumers’ perception of products [30][31]. Specifically, since cultural media have a notable impact on consumers, the halo effect on the culture of these origin countries could theoretically influence consumer behaviors. For instance, positive cultural halo effects in the Japanese import market has been recorded, indicating that cultural products import has significant positive effects on certain types of consumer products of the same country of origin and negative effects on products of different origin [32]. The listed literature provides an innovative perspective for countries that export products. It can be deduced that combining cultural products and other products could maximize halo effect returns, but no empirical studies have been done to vindicate that idea. Therefore, future investigations should be targeted at conducting empirical studies on the methods of generating larger halos, and to expand the subjects from Japan to other countries.
In general, halo effect can be both directly and indirectly beneficial for corporations’ businesses, thus knowing the exact methods to generate the halo is in urgent need. Researchers profess that sponsorship has a positive effect on brand’s image and reputation [33][34][35]. They also conclude that sponsorship projects with high sponsorship awareness and high goodwill could generate the largest halo effect return for companies. However, these studies only focus on large-scaled B2C companies. Whether the same strategy is appropriate for small-scaled B2C companies or B2B companies still needs to be examined in future studies.
4.2. Halo in Corporate Management
As one of the first fields to study halo effect, HRM has long been beset by halo effect. Until contemporary days, precluding halo effect from influencing HRM processes has still been a major task for practitioners. Among all HRM practices employment interview and performance rating are the major focuses, since rating is the pivotal part of them.
4.2.1. Employment Interview
Although some scholars have proven the halo effect to increase the accuracy in ratings under certain circumstances, most companies still regard the halo effect as a rating error in their recruitment process, because the halo effect might lead to wrong employment and thus lower corporate performance [36]. Thus, eliminating the halo in employment interviews is a key mission for scholars and managers.
Existing literature has confirmed the halo effect in employment interviews and has identified its influencing factors. Back in the 1950s, scholars had already discovered systematic and sporadic halo in employment interviews [37]. To help corporations eliminate halo, multiple experiments are conducted to explore the factors altering halo in the interviewing process. According to existing studies, the interview rating methods, interviewers’ training methods, the type of expectations(favorable or unfavorable) an interviewer possesses of an applicant, applicant’s race and accent and interview strategies are the major factors that could influence the halo in the interviewing process [38][39][40][41][42][43]. After the influencing factors of halo are detected, scholars begin to find stable methods of minimizing halo in employment interview.
Different scholars propound various methods to minimize halo in employment interviews. Latham et al., claim that both workshop and group discussion could eliminate the halo effect in job interview [39]. While workshop appears to be more effective in reducing the halo effect, the expense is also higher, so enterprises could choose a form of interview according to the company’s conditions. However, Kiker finds a paradox in that a rating strategy that raises internal consistency reliability also lowers interrater reliability [43]. Thus, no perfect strategy has been proposed. Also, a significant limitation of studies on halo in employment interview is that most of the conclusions are deduced from experimental data rather than real-life scenarios. Thus, these conclusions might not accurately reflect how the halo effect emerges in employment interviews. Future studies should attempt to collect more data from real companies and enrich the halo literature.
4.2.2. Performance Rating
Performance rating in corporations has always been a pivotal part of HRM, since illusory halo could damage organizational justice. The confirmation of the halo effect in performance rating is significant. Though Thorndike proposes the concept of the halo effect, he mainly describes and defines the phenomenon [1]. Later, many scholars successfully prove the halo effect in performance rating, further accentuating the importance of halo effect [44][45][46].
Subsequently, further studies are conducted to identify the influencing factors of the halo effect on performance ratings. Studies indicate that when raters lack information about employees performance conditions, their judgment is more inclined to be altered by the halo effect, thus requiring managers to monitor employees’ performance at the workplace, which supports many previous scholars’ expectations [8][47][48][49]. However, such a perspective is questioned by Jacobs and Kozlowski, whose study shows that the halo in ratings increases with increasing familiarity [50]. The reason for such inconsistency is still unknown, indicating that future studies should attempt to discover the mediators and moderators between familiarity and the halo effect.
Besides the inconsistency in familiarity, debates occur over the best method to eliminate halo in performance appraisal. Landy et al. propose that among the 4 classic methods of controlling rating errors, residual rating scores may be used as supplements to raw scores so that halo effect could be eliminated effectively in performance rating [51]. Similarly, King et al. believe that the use of a forced-choice rating scale could also decrease halo effect in rating [52]. However, even without a consensus on the methods to eliminate halo, at the practical level, decreasing halo by training managers has been proven to be effective [53][47].
However, at the practical level, performance rating is still facing three major problems. First, although Nathan and Murphy have professed that halo in performance rating does not equal inaccuracy, and that it might show real accuracy [54][36]. However, scholars have yet to distinguish valid halo and invalid halo in performance rating. Moreover, with more employees learning about halo effect, they start to develop impression management strategies and have successfully obtained higher performance ratings under certain circumstances [55]. Future studies should focus on more exact moderators and mediators that allow employees to successfully utilize impression strategies. Lastly, previous studies hardly connect performance rating to compensation management, which is another pivotal HRM process. In recent years, some scholars have realized the lack of investigation in this field and have proven that halo has a significant impact on managers’ salary decisions [56]. However, it is merely a beginning. More studies are required to be conducted to enrich the halo effect literature and eventually help enterprises with compensation management practice.
5. Conclusion
According to existing literature, the following conclusions can be drawn. At corporate level, the halo effect could be beneficial for businesses, and be challenging for management. The halo effect has proven to be beneficial for brand marketing both online and offline, which promotes sales in a notable way. Nevertheless, more studies still need to be done, so that different types of enterprises could generate large halos for higher returns. However, the halo effect has been a great problem for HRM. Both employment interview and performance ratings have developed multiple methods to avoid halo effect from influencing organizational justice and corporate performance. However, it is merely a beginning. More studies are required to be conducted in order to enrich the halo effect literature and eventually help enterprises with compensation management practice. This essay reviews how the halo effect is developed and measured, which in turn inspires corporations to notice the potential benefits and threats of this heuristic. In conclusion, while the definition and measurement of the halo effect have been fully developed throughout the century, some core problems have still been unsolved. For instance, a stable method to distinguish true halo and illusory halo has not been developed, which leads to many management problems. One of the most essential tasks for scholars is to solve this problem in the near future.
References
[1]. Thorndike EL. A constant error in psychological ratings [J]. Journal of Applied Psychology. 4(1):25-29. 1920.
[2]. Johnson DM, Vidulich RN. Experimental manipulation of the halo effect. Journal of Applied Psychology. 40(2):130-134-134. 1956.
[3]. Johnson DM. Reanalysis of experimental halo effects. Journal of Applied Psychology. 47:46-47. 1963.
[4]. Saal FE, Downey RG, Lahey MA. Rating the ratings: Assessing the psychometric quality of rating data. Psychological Bulletin. 88(2):413-428-428. 1980.
[5]. Cooper WH. Ubiquitous halo. Psychological Bulletin. 90:218-244. 1981.
[6]. Feldman JM. Beyond attribution theory: Cognitive processes in performance appraisal. Journal of Applied Psychology. 66(2):127-148-148. 1981.
[7]. Kozlowski, S. W J., Kirsch, M. P. The systematic distortion hypothesis, halo, and accuracy: An individual-level analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology. 72: 252-261. 1981.
[8]. Kozlowski, S.W J., Kirsch, M.P, Chao, G.T. Job knowledge, grate familiarity, conceptual similarity, and halo error: An exploration. Journal of Applied Psychology. 71: 45-49. 1986.
[9]. Lance CE, Woehr DJ. Statistical control of halo: clarification from two cognitive models of the performance appraisal process. Journal of applied psychology. 71(4):679-685. 1986.
[10]. Nathan BR, Lord RG. Cognitive Categorization and Dimensional Schemata: A Process Approach to the Study of Halo in Performance Ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology. 68(1):102-114. 1983.
[11]. Symonds PM. Notes on rating. Journal of Applied Psychology. 9(2):188-195. 1925.
[12]. Kaplan MF. Context-induced shifts in personality trait evaluation: A comment on the evaluative halo effect and meaning change interpretations. Psychological Bulletin. 81:891-895. 1974.
[13]. Murphy KR, Anhalt RL. Is halo error a property of the rater, ratees, or the specific behaviors observed? Journal of applied psychology. 77(4):494-500. 1992.
[14]. Balzer WK, Sulsky LM. Halo and performance appraisal research: a critical examination. Journal of Applied Psychology. v77(n6). 1992.
[15]. Fox S, Bizman A, Herrmann E. The halo effect: Is it a unitary concept? Journal of Occupational Psychology. 56(4):289-296. 1983.
[16]. Nathan BR. The halo effect: It is a unitary concept! A reply to Fox et al. (1983). Journal of Occupational Psychology. 59(1):41-44. 1986.
[17]. Fox S, Bizman A, Hoffman M. The halo effect: It really isn’t unitary: A rejoinder to Nathan (1986). Journal of Occupational Psychology. 62(2):183-188. 1989.
[18]. Bingham WV. Halo, invalid and valid. Journal of Applied Psychology. 23:221-228. 1939.
[19]. Jin K-Y, Chiu MM. A mixture Rasch facets model for rater’s illusory halo effects. Behavior Research Methods. 54(6):2750-2764. 2022.
[20]. Berry LL, Parasuraman A. Marketing services: competing through quality. New York: Free Press. 151-156. 1991.
[21]. Wilkie WL, Mccann JM, Reibstein DJ. Halo effects in brand belief measurement: Implications for attitude model development. Advances in Consumer Research. 1: 280 - 290. 1974.
[22]. Beckwith NE, Kassarjian HH, Lehmann DR. Halo Effects in Marketing Research: Review and Prognosis. Advances in Consumer Research. 5(1):465-467. 1978.
[23]. Beckwith NE, Lehmann DR. The Importance of Halo Effects in Multi-Attribute Attitude Models. Journal of Marketing Research (JMR). 12(3):265-275. 1975.
[24]. Beckwith, N. E., & Lehmann, D. R. Halo effects in multiattribute attitude models:An appraisal of some unresolved issues. Journal of Marketing Research, 13(4), 418-421. (1976).
[25]. Johansson JK, MacLachlan DL, Yalch RF. Halo Effects in Multiattribute Attitude Models: Some Unresolved Issues. Journal of Marketing Research (JMR). 13(4):414-417. 1976.
[26]. Keller KL. Conceptualizing, Measuring, Managing Customer-Based Brand Equity. Journal of Marketing. 57(1):1-22. 1993.
[27]. Leuthesser L, Kohli CS, Harich KR. Brand equity: the halo effect measure. European Journal of Marketing. v29(n4). 1995.
[28]. Degeratu AM. Consumer choice behavior in online and traditional supermarkets: The effects of brand name, price, and other search attributes. International journal of research in marketing. 17(1):55-78. 2000.
[29]. Kwon W-S, Lennon SJ. What induces online loyalty? Online versus offline brand images. Journal of Business Research. 62(5):557-564. 2009.
[30]. Bilkey, W. J., & Nes, E. Country-of-origin effects on product evaluations. Journal of international business studies, 13, 89-100. (1982).
[31]. Erickson GM, Johansson JK, Chao P. Image Variables in Multi-Attribute Product Evaluations: Country-of-Origin Effects. Journal of Consumer Research. 11(2):694-699. 1984.
[32]. Lee SH, Han DB, Nayga RM. Cultural inflow effects on Japanese import demand for consumer products: importance of halo effects. Journal of the Asia Pacific Economy. 19(3):506-521. 2014.
[33]. Polonsky MJ, Speed R. Linking sponsorship and cause related marketing. European Journal of Marketing. 35(11/12): 1361-1389. 2001.
[34]. Rifon NJ, Choi SM, Trimble CS, Li H. Congruence Effects in Sponsorship: The Mediating Role of Sponsor Credibility and Consumer Attributions of Sponsor Motive. Journal of Advertising. 33(1): 30-42. 2004.
[35]. Vance L, Raciti MM, Lawley M. Beyond brand exposure: measuring the sponsorship halo effect. Measuring Business Excellence. 20(3):1-14. 2016.
[36]. Murphy KR, Jako RA, Anhalt RL. Nature and consequences of halo error: a critical analysis. Journal of applied psychology. 78(2):218-225. 1993.
[37]. Crissy WJE, Regan JJ. Halo in the employment interview. Journal of Applied Psychology. 35(5):338-341. 1951.
[38]. Herbert G. Heneman, Donald P. Schwab, Dennis L. Huett, John J. Ford. Interviewer validity as a function of interview structure, biographical data, and interviewee order. Journal of Applied Psychology. 60:748-753. 1975.
[39]. Latham GP, Wexley KN, Pursell ED. Training Managers to Minimize Rating Errors in the Observation of Behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology. 60(5):550-555. 1975.
[40]. Tucker DH, Rowe PM. Relationship Between Expectancy, Causal Attributions, and Final Hiring Decisions in the Employment Interview. Journal of Applied Psychology. 64(1):27-34. 1979.
[41]. Huang MS, Singer AE. Ethnic group stereotyping by police and university students in New Zealand. The Journal of psychology. 116(1):89-93. 1984.
[42]. Singer M, Eder GS. Effects of Ethnicity, Accent, and Job Status on Selection Decisions. International Journal of Psychology. 24(1):13. 1989.
[43]. Kiker DS, Motowidlo SJ. Effects of Rating Strategy on Interdimensional Variance, Reliability, and Validity of Interview Ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology.83(5):763-768. 1998.
[44]. Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. The halo effect: Evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 35(4):250-256-256. 1977.
[45]. Wetzel CG, Wilson TD, Kort J. The halo effect revisited: Forewarned is not forearmed. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 17(4):427-439-439. 1981.
[46]. Belle N, Cantarelli P, Belardinelli P. Cognitive Biases in Performance Appraisal: Experimental Evidence on Anchoring and Halo Effects With Public Sector Managers and Employees. Review of Public Personnel Administration. 37(3). 2017.
[47]. Brown EM. Influence of Training, Method, and Relationship of the Halo Effect. Journal of Applied Psychology. 52(3):195-199. 1968.
[48]. Borman WC. Exploring Upper Limits of Reliability and Validity in Job Performance Ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology. 63(2):135-144. 1978.
[49]. Campbell JP, Dunnette MD, Arvey RD, Hellervik LV. The Development and Evaluation of Behaviorally Based Rating Scales. Journal of Applied Psychology. 57(1):15-22. 1973.
[50]. Jacobs R, Kozlowski SWJ. A Closer Look at Halo Error in Performance Ratings. Academy of Management Journal. 28(1):201-212. 1985.
[51]. Landy FJ, Barnes-Farrell JL, Vance RJ, Steele JW. Statistical Control of Halo Error in Performance Ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology. 65(5):501-506. 1980.
[52]. King LM, Hunter JE, Schmidt FL. Halo in a Multidimensional Forced-Choice Performance Evaluation Scale. Journal of Applied Psychology. 65(5):507-516. 1980.
[53]. Taylor EK, Hastman R. Relation of Format and Administration to the Characteristics of Graphic Rating Scales. Personnel Psychology. 9(2):181-206. 1956.
[54]. Nathan BR, Tippins N. The Consequences of Halo “Error” in Performance Ratings: A Field Study of the Moderating Effect of Halo on Test Validation Results. Journal of Applied Psychology. 75(3):290-296. 1990.
[55]. Huang GH, Niu XY, Hailin ZH, Ashford SJ, Lee C. Reducing Job Insecurity and Increasing Performance Ratings: Does Impression Management Matter? Journal of applied psychology. 98(5):852-862. 2013.
[56]. Maske MK, Sohn M, Hirsch B. How managerial accountability mitigates a halo effect in managers’ ex-post bonus adjustments. Management Accounting Research. 51. 2021.
Cite this article
Yao,Y. (2023). A Review of the Halo Effect and Its Applications at the Corporate Level. Advances in Economics, Management and Political Sciences,42,7-13.
Data availability
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study will be available from the authors upon reasonable request.
Disclaimer/Publisher's Note
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of EWA Publishing and/or the editor(s). EWA Publishing and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
About volume
Volume title: Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Economic Management and Green Development
© 2024 by the author(s). Licensee EWA Publishing, Oxford, UK. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license. Authors who
publish this series agree to the following terms:
1. Authors retain copyright and grant the series right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgment of the work's authorship and initial publication in this
series.
2. Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the series's published
version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgment of its initial
publication in this series.
3. Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and
during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See
Open access policy for details).
References
[1]. Thorndike EL. A constant error in psychological ratings [J]. Journal of Applied Psychology. 4(1):25-29. 1920.
[2]. Johnson DM, Vidulich RN. Experimental manipulation of the halo effect. Journal of Applied Psychology. 40(2):130-134-134. 1956.
[3]. Johnson DM. Reanalysis of experimental halo effects. Journal of Applied Psychology. 47:46-47. 1963.
[4]. Saal FE, Downey RG, Lahey MA. Rating the ratings: Assessing the psychometric quality of rating data. Psychological Bulletin. 88(2):413-428-428. 1980.
[5]. Cooper WH. Ubiquitous halo. Psychological Bulletin. 90:218-244. 1981.
[6]. Feldman JM. Beyond attribution theory: Cognitive processes in performance appraisal. Journal of Applied Psychology. 66(2):127-148-148. 1981.
[7]. Kozlowski, S. W J., Kirsch, M. P. The systematic distortion hypothesis, halo, and accuracy: An individual-level analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology. 72: 252-261. 1981.
[8]. Kozlowski, S.W J., Kirsch, M.P, Chao, G.T. Job knowledge, grate familiarity, conceptual similarity, and halo error: An exploration. Journal of Applied Psychology. 71: 45-49. 1986.
[9]. Lance CE, Woehr DJ. Statistical control of halo: clarification from two cognitive models of the performance appraisal process. Journal of applied psychology. 71(4):679-685. 1986.
[10]. Nathan BR, Lord RG. Cognitive Categorization and Dimensional Schemata: A Process Approach to the Study of Halo in Performance Ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology. 68(1):102-114. 1983.
[11]. Symonds PM. Notes on rating. Journal of Applied Psychology. 9(2):188-195. 1925.
[12]. Kaplan MF. Context-induced shifts in personality trait evaluation: A comment on the evaluative halo effect and meaning change interpretations. Psychological Bulletin. 81:891-895. 1974.
[13]. Murphy KR, Anhalt RL. Is halo error a property of the rater, ratees, or the specific behaviors observed? Journal of applied psychology. 77(4):494-500. 1992.
[14]. Balzer WK, Sulsky LM. Halo and performance appraisal research: a critical examination. Journal of Applied Psychology. v77(n6). 1992.
[15]. Fox S, Bizman A, Herrmann E. The halo effect: Is it a unitary concept? Journal of Occupational Psychology. 56(4):289-296. 1983.
[16]. Nathan BR. The halo effect: It is a unitary concept! A reply to Fox et al. (1983). Journal of Occupational Psychology. 59(1):41-44. 1986.
[17]. Fox S, Bizman A, Hoffman M. The halo effect: It really isn’t unitary: A rejoinder to Nathan (1986). Journal of Occupational Psychology. 62(2):183-188. 1989.
[18]. Bingham WV. Halo, invalid and valid. Journal of Applied Psychology. 23:221-228. 1939.
[19]. Jin K-Y, Chiu MM. A mixture Rasch facets model for rater’s illusory halo effects. Behavior Research Methods. 54(6):2750-2764. 2022.
[20]. Berry LL, Parasuraman A. Marketing services: competing through quality. New York: Free Press. 151-156. 1991.
[21]. Wilkie WL, Mccann JM, Reibstein DJ. Halo effects in brand belief measurement: Implications for attitude model development. Advances in Consumer Research. 1: 280 - 290. 1974.
[22]. Beckwith NE, Kassarjian HH, Lehmann DR. Halo Effects in Marketing Research: Review and Prognosis. Advances in Consumer Research. 5(1):465-467. 1978.
[23]. Beckwith NE, Lehmann DR. The Importance of Halo Effects in Multi-Attribute Attitude Models. Journal of Marketing Research (JMR). 12(3):265-275. 1975.
[24]. Beckwith, N. E., & Lehmann, D. R. Halo effects in multiattribute attitude models:An appraisal of some unresolved issues. Journal of Marketing Research, 13(4), 418-421. (1976).
[25]. Johansson JK, MacLachlan DL, Yalch RF. Halo Effects in Multiattribute Attitude Models: Some Unresolved Issues. Journal of Marketing Research (JMR). 13(4):414-417. 1976.
[26]. Keller KL. Conceptualizing, Measuring, Managing Customer-Based Brand Equity. Journal of Marketing. 57(1):1-22. 1993.
[27]. Leuthesser L, Kohli CS, Harich KR. Brand equity: the halo effect measure. European Journal of Marketing. v29(n4). 1995.
[28]. Degeratu AM. Consumer choice behavior in online and traditional supermarkets: The effects of brand name, price, and other search attributes. International journal of research in marketing. 17(1):55-78. 2000.
[29]. Kwon W-S, Lennon SJ. What induces online loyalty? Online versus offline brand images. Journal of Business Research. 62(5):557-564. 2009.
[30]. Bilkey, W. J., & Nes, E. Country-of-origin effects on product evaluations. Journal of international business studies, 13, 89-100. (1982).
[31]. Erickson GM, Johansson JK, Chao P. Image Variables in Multi-Attribute Product Evaluations: Country-of-Origin Effects. Journal of Consumer Research. 11(2):694-699. 1984.
[32]. Lee SH, Han DB, Nayga RM. Cultural inflow effects on Japanese import demand for consumer products: importance of halo effects. Journal of the Asia Pacific Economy. 19(3):506-521. 2014.
[33]. Polonsky MJ, Speed R. Linking sponsorship and cause related marketing. European Journal of Marketing. 35(11/12): 1361-1389. 2001.
[34]. Rifon NJ, Choi SM, Trimble CS, Li H. Congruence Effects in Sponsorship: The Mediating Role of Sponsor Credibility and Consumer Attributions of Sponsor Motive. Journal of Advertising. 33(1): 30-42. 2004.
[35]. Vance L, Raciti MM, Lawley M. Beyond brand exposure: measuring the sponsorship halo effect. Measuring Business Excellence. 20(3):1-14. 2016.
[36]. Murphy KR, Jako RA, Anhalt RL. Nature and consequences of halo error: a critical analysis. Journal of applied psychology. 78(2):218-225. 1993.
[37]. Crissy WJE, Regan JJ. Halo in the employment interview. Journal of Applied Psychology. 35(5):338-341. 1951.
[38]. Herbert G. Heneman, Donald P. Schwab, Dennis L. Huett, John J. Ford. Interviewer validity as a function of interview structure, biographical data, and interviewee order. Journal of Applied Psychology. 60:748-753. 1975.
[39]. Latham GP, Wexley KN, Pursell ED. Training Managers to Minimize Rating Errors in the Observation of Behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology. 60(5):550-555. 1975.
[40]. Tucker DH, Rowe PM. Relationship Between Expectancy, Causal Attributions, and Final Hiring Decisions in the Employment Interview. Journal of Applied Psychology. 64(1):27-34. 1979.
[41]. Huang MS, Singer AE. Ethnic group stereotyping by police and university students in New Zealand. The Journal of psychology. 116(1):89-93. 1984.
[42]. Singer M, Eder GS. Effects of Ethnicity, Accent, and Job Status on Selection Decisions. International Journal of Psychology. 24(1):13. 1989.
[43]. Kiker DS, Motowidlo SJ. Effects of Rating Strategy on Interdimensional Variance, Reliability, and Validity of Interview Ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology.83(5):763-768. 1998.
[44]. Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. The halo effect: Evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 35(4):250-256-256. 1977.
[45]. Wetzel CG, Wilson TD, Kort J. The halo effect revisited: Forewarned is not forearmed. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 17(4):427-439-439. 1981.
[46]. Belle N, Cantarelli P, Belardinelli P. Cognitive Biases in Performance Appraisal: Experimental Evidence on Anchoring and Halo Effects With Public Sector Managers and Employees. Review of Public Personnel Administration. 37(3). 2017.
[47]. Brown EM. Influence of Training, Method, and Relationship of the Halo Effect. Journal of Applied Psychology. 52(3):195-199. 1968.
[48]. Borman WC. Exploring Upper Limits of Reliability and Validity in Job Performance Ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology. 63(2):135-144. 1978.
[49]. Campbell JP, Dunnette MD, Arvey RD, Hellervik LV. The Development and Evaluation of Behaviorally Based Rating Scales. Journal of Applied Psychology. 57(1):15-22. 1973.
[50]. Jacobs R, Kozlowski SWJ. A Closer Look at Halo Error in Performance Ratings. Academy of Management Journal. 28(1):201-212. 1985.
[51]. Landy FJ, Barnes-Farrell JL, Vance RJ, Steele JW. Statistical Control of Halo Error in Performance Ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology. 65(5):501-506. 1980.
[52]. King LM, Hunter JE, Schmidt FL. Halo in a Multidimensional Forced-Choice Performance Evaluation Scale. Journal of Applied Psychology. 65(5):507-516. 1980.
[53]. Taylor EK, Hastman R. Relation of Format and Administration to the Characteristics of Graphic Rating Scales. Personnel Psychology. 9(2):181-206. 1956.
[54]. Nathan BR, Tippins N. The Consequences of Halo “Error” in Performance Ratings: A Field Study of the Moderating Effect of Halo on Test Validation Results. Journal of Applied Psychology. 75(3):290-296. 1990.
[55]. Huang GH, Niu XY, Hailin ZH, Ashford SJ, Lee C. Reducing Job Insecurity and Increasing Performance Ratings: Does Impression Management Matter? Journal of applied psychology. 98(5):852-862. 2013.
[56]. Maske MK, Sohn M, Hirsch B. How managerial accountability mitigates a halo effect in managers’ ex-post bonus adjustments. Management Accounting Research. 51. 2021.