References
[1]. Schwab, J. F., & Lew-Williams, C. (2016). Language learning, socioeconomic status, and child-directed speech. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 7(4), 264–275. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1393
[2]. Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–2152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145
[3]. Ramírez-Esparza, N., García-Sierra, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2014). Look who’s talking: speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development. Developmental Science, 17(6), 880–891. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12172 6.
[4]. Anderson, D. R., & Evans, M. K. (2001). Peril and potential of media for infants and toddlers. Zero to Three: National Center for Infants, Toddlers, and Families, 22(2), 10–16.
[5]. Pempek, T. A., Kirkorian, H. L., & Anderson, D. R. (2014). The Effects of Background Television on the Quantity and Quality of Child-Directed Speech by Parents. Journal of Children and Media, 8(3), 211–222. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2014.920715
[6]. Reed, J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Learning on hold: Cell phones sidetrack parent-child interactions. Developmental Psychology, 53(8), 1428–1436. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000292
[7]. Przybylski, A. K., & Weinstein, N. (2012). Can you connect with me now? How the presence of mobile communication technology influences face-to-face conversation quality. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 30(3), 237–246. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407512453827
[8]. Misra, S., Cheng, L., Genevie, J., & Yuan, M. (2014). The iPhone Effect. Environment and Behavior, 48(2), 275–298. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916514539755
[9]. Van Dam, J., Kass, S. J., & VanWormer, L. (2019). The effects of passive mobile phone interaction on situation awareness and driving performance. Journal of Transportation Safety & Security, 12(8), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/19439962.2018.1564947
[10]. Corkin, M. T., Henderson, A. M. E., Peterson, E. R., Kennedy- Costantini, S., Sharplin, H. S., & Morrison, S. (2021). Associations between technoference, quality of parent-infant interactions, and infants’ vocabulary development. Infant Behavior and Development, 64, 101611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101611
[11]. Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How Children Use Input to Acquire a Lexicon. Child Development, 73(2), 418–433. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00415
Cite this article
Li,K. (2023). How do Audible Message Notifications Influence the Quantity and Quality of Child-Directed Speech from Parents?. Lecture Notes in Education Psychology and Public Media,18,91-95.
Data availability
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study will be available from the authors upon reasonable request.
Disclaimer/Publisher's Note
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of EWA Publishing and/or the editor(s). EWA Publishing and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
About volume
Volume title: Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Educational Innovation and Philosophical Inquiries
© 2024 by the author(s). Licensee EWA Publishing, Oxford, UK. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license. Authors who
publish this series agree to the following terms:
1. Authors retain copyright and grant the series right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgment of the work's authorship and initial publication in this
series.
2. Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the series's published
version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgment of its initial
publication in this series.
3. Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and
during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See
Open access policy for details).
References
[1]. Schwab, J. F., & Lew-Williams, C. (2016). Language learning, socioeconomic status, and child-directed speech. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 7(4), 264–275. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1393
[2]. Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–2152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145
[3]. Ramírez-Esparza, N., García-Sierra, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2014). Look who’s talking: speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development. Developmental Science, 17(6), 880–891. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12172 6.
[4]. Anderson, D. R., & Evans, M. K. (2001). Peril and potential of media for infants and toddlers. Zero to Three: National Center for Infants, Toddlers, and Families, 22(2), 10–16.
[5]. Pempek, T. A., Kirkorian, H. L., & Anderson, D. R. (2014). The Effects of Background Television on the Quantity and Quality of Child-Directed Speech by Parents. Journal of Children and Media, 8(3), 211–222. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2014.920715
[6]. Reed, J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Learning on hold: Cell phones sidetrack parent-child interactions. Developmental Psychology, 53(8), 1428–1436. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000292
[7]. Przybylski, A. K., & Weinstein, N. (2012). Can you connect with me now? How the presence of mobile communication technology influences face-to-face conversation quality. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 30(3), 237–246. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407512453827
[8]. Misra, S., Cheng, L., Genevie, J., & Yuan, M. (2014). The iPhone Effect. Environment and Behavior, 48(2), 275–298. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916514539755
[9]. Van Dam, J., Kass, S. J., & VanWormer, L. (2019). The effects of passive mobile phone interaction on situation awareness and driving performance. Journal of Transportation Safety & Security, 12(8), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/19439962.2018.1564947
[10]. Corkin, M. T., Henderson, A. M. E., Peterson, E. R., Kennedy- Costantini, S., Sharplin, H. S., & Morrison, S. (2021). Associations between technoference, quality of parent-infant interactions, and infants’ vocabulary development. Infant Behavior and Development, 64, 101611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101611
[11]. Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How Children Use Input to Acquire a Lexicon. Child Development, 73(2), 418–433. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00415