
The Role of Spontaneous Thoughts in Human Cognition
- 1 College of Biological Science, University of California Davis, Davis, California, US
* Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Abstract
Spontaneous thought is a cognitive process that ubiquitously happens in daily life. Humans spend 30-50% of their lives in spontaneous thinking, but the impact and consequence of spontaneous thought have not been widely studied in cognitive neuroscience. It is crucial to expand the research involving spontaneous thought since it benefits human's memory encoding and decision-making abilities. This review has two main aims: the first is to explore the cognitive impact of goal-directed and spontaneous thought; the second is to discover the benefits of spontaneous thought to humans. This paper concludes that spontaneous thought has the role of improving memory encoding and facilitating future planning.
Keywords
[1]. Blondé, P., Makowski, D., Sperduti, M., & Piolino, P. (2021). In Medio Stat Virtus: Intermediate levels of mind wandering improve episodic memory encoding in a virtual environment. Psychological Research, 85(4), 1613–1625.
[2]. Chaieb, L., Hoppe, C., & Fell, J. (2022). Mind wandering and depression: A status report. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 133, 104505.
[3]. Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., & Conway, A. M. (2009). Happiness unpacked: Positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience. Emotion, 9(3), 361–368.
[4]. Crosswell, A. D., Coccia, M., & Epel, E. S. (2020). Mind wandering and stress: When you don’t like the present moment. Emotion, 20(3), 403–412.
[5]. D’Argembeau, A., Renaud, O., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Frequency, characteristics and functions of future-oriented thoughts in daily life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(1), 96– 103.
[6]. Devos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Implicit Self and Identity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1001(1), 177–211.
[7]. Dijksterhuis, A., & Strick, M. (2016). A Case for Thinking Without Consciousness. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(1), 117–132.
[8]. Dutt, S., Li, Y., Mather, M., Nation, D. A., & for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. (2021). Brainstem substructures and cognition in prodromal Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 15(5), 2572–2582.
[9]. Gearin, B., Fien, H., & Nelson, N. J. (2018). Mind wandering: A potentially generative idea for understanding the socioeconomic status academic achievement gap. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 4(2), 138–152.
[10]. Gonzalez-Castillo, J., Caballero-Gaudes, C., Topolski, N., Handwerker, D. A., Pereira, F., & Bandettini, P. A. (2019). Imaging the spontaneous flow of thought: Distinct periods of cognition contribute to dynamic functional connectivity during rest. NeuroImage, 202, 116129.
[11]. He, H., Hu, L., Zhang, X., & Qiu, J. (2021). Pleasantness of mind wandering is positively associated with focus back effort in daily life: Evidence from resting state fMRI. Brain and Cognition, 150, 105731.
[12]. Hohwy, J., Paton, B., & Palmer, C. (2016). Distrusting the present. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 315–335.
[13]. Kam, J. W. Y., Mittner, M., & Knight, R. T. (2022). Mind-wandering: Mechanistic insights from lesion, tDCS, and iEEG. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, S1364661321003132.
[14]. Kamiński, J., Sullivan, S., Chung, J. M., Ross, I. B., Mamelak, A. N., & Rutishauser, U. (2017). Persistently active neurons in human medial frontal and medial temporal lobe support working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 20(4), 590–601.
[15]. Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A Wandering Mind Is an Unhappy Mind. Science, 330(6006), 932–932.
[16]. Krakau, S., Chaieb, L., Helmstaedter, C., Wrede, R., & Fell, J. (2020). Reduced past‐oriented mind wandering in left compared to right medial temporal lobe epilepsy. European Journal of Neuroscience, 52(5), 3411–3418.
[17]. Luelsberg, F., Krakau, S., Chaieb, L., Witt, J.-A., von Wrede, R., Fell, J., & Helmstaedter, C. (2022). Neuropsychological features of mind wandering in left-, right- and extra temporal lobe epilepsy. Seizure, 95, 50–55.
[18]. Marjanović, M., Comoutos, N., & Papaioannou, A. (2019). The relationships between perceived motivational climate, achievement goals and self-talk in physical education: Testing the mediating role of achievement goals and self-talk. Motivation and Emotion, 43(4), 592–609.
[19]. Marks, S. M., Lockhart, S. N., Baker, S. L., & Jagust, W. J. (2017). Tau and β-Amyloid Are Associated with Medial Temporal Lobe Structure, Function, and Memory Encoding in Normal Aging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(12), 3192–3201.
[20]. McCormick, C., Rosenthal, C. R., Miller, T. D., & Maguire, E. A. (2018). Mind-Wandering in People with Hippocampal Damage. The Journal of Neuroscience, 38(11), 2745–2754.
[21]. McWhinney, S. R., Tremblay, A., Boe, S. G., & Bardouille, T. (2018). The impact of goal- oriented task design on neurofeedback learning for brain–computer interface control. Medical & Biological Engineering & Computing, 56(2), 201–210.
[22]. Mildner, J. N., & Tamir, D. I. (2019). Spontaneous Thought as an Unconstrained Memory Process. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(11), 763–777.
[23]. Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wandering: Finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12(3), 442–448.
[24]. Perogamvros, L., Baird, B., Seibold, M., Riedner, B., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2017). The Phenomenal Contents and Neural Correlates of Spontaneous Thoughts across Wakefulness, NREM Sleep, and REM Sleep. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(10), 1766–1777.
[25]. Pezzulo, G., & Castelfranchi, C. (2009). Thinking as the control of imagination: A conceptual framework for goal-directed systems. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 73(4), 559–577.
[26]. Qiu, Y., Fan, Z., Zhong, M., Yang, J., Wu, K., Huiqing, H., Zhang, R., Guo, Y., Lee, T. M. C., & Huang, R. (2022). Brain activation elicited by acute stress: An ALE meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 132, 706–724.
[27]. Schimmenti, A., Sideli, L., La Marca, L., Gori, A., & Terrone, G. (2020). Reliability, Validity, and Factor Structure of the Maladaptive Daydreaming Scale (MDS–16) in an Italian Sample. Journal of Personality Assessment, 102(5), 689–701.
[28]. Schmorrow, D. D., & Fidopiastis, C. M. (Eds.). (2017). Augmented Cognition. Neurocognition and Machine Learning: 11th International Conference, AC 2017, Held as Part of HCI International 2017, Vancouver, BC, Canada, July 9-14, 2017, Proceedings, Part I (Vol. 10284). Springer International Publishing.
[29]. Stawarczyk, D. (2018). Phenomenological Properties of Mind-Wandering and Daydreaming (K. Christoff & K. C. R. Fox, Eds.; Vol. 1). Oxford University Press.
[30]. Tomescu, M. I., Papasteri, C. C., Sofonea, A., Boldasu, R., Kebets, V., Pistol, C. A. D., Poalelungi, C., Benescu, V., Podina, I. R., Nedelcea, C. I., Berceanu, A. I., & Carcea, I. (2022). Spontaneous thought and microstate activity modulation by social imitation. NeuroImage, 249, 118878.
[31]. Yan, Y., Aierken, A., Wang, C., Song, D., Ni, J., Wang, Z., Quan, Z., & Qing, H. (2022). A potential biomarker of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: The olfactory dysfunction and its pathogenesis-based neural circuitry impairments. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 132, 857–869.
[32]. Zanesco, A. P., Denkova, E., & Jha, A. P. (2021). Associations between self-reported spontaneous thought and temporal sequences of EEG microstates. Brain and Cognition, 150, 105696.
Cite this article
Huang,S. (2023). The Role of Spontaneous Thoughts in Human Cognition. Theoretical and Natural Science,3,38-43.
Data availability
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study will be available from the authors upon reasonable request.
Disclaimer/Publisher's Note
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of EWA Publishing and/or the editor(s). EWA Publishing and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
About volume
Volume title: Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Biological Engineering and Medical Science (ICBioMed 2022), Part I
© 2024 by the author(s). Licensee EWA Publishing, Oxford, UK. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license. Authors who
publish this series agree to the following terms:
1. Authors retain copyright and grant the series right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgment of the work's authorship and initial publication in this
series.
2. Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the series's published
version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgment of its initial
publication in this series.
3. Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and
during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See
Open access policy for details).