References
[1]. Salehinejad, M.A., et al. (2017) Cognitive Control Dysfunction in Emotion Dysregulation and Psychopathology of Major Depression (MD): Evidence from Transcranial Brain Stimulation of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex (DLPFC). Journal of affective disorders, 210, 241.
[2]. Joormann, J. and Ema, T. (2015) Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression: Examining Cognitive Control and Emotion Regulation. Current opinion in psychology, 4, 86.
[3]. Grahek, I., et al. (2018) Cognitive Control in Depression: Toward Clinical Models Informed by Cognitive Neuroscience. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(4), 464.
[4]. Miyake, A. and Naomi, P.F. (2012) The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Current directions in psychological science : a journal of the American Psychological Society, 21(1), 8.
[5]. Koster, E.H.W., Evi de, L. and Rudi de, R. (2013) Rumination Is Characterized by Valence-Specific Impairments in Switching of Attention. Acta psychologica, 144(3), 563.
[6]. Whitmer, A.J. and Ian, H.G. (2012) Switching and Backward Inhibition in Major Depressive Disorder: The Role of Rumination. Journal of abnormal psychology, 121(3), 570.
[7]. Kaiser, R.H., et al. (2015) Distracted and down: Neural Mechanisms of Affective Interference in Subclinical Depression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(5), 654.
[8]. Auerbach, R.P., et al. (2013) Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms Underlying Cognitive Vulnerability Models of Depression. Journal of psychotherapy integration, 23(3), 222.
[9]. Bar, M. (2009) A Cognitive Neuroscience Hypothesis of Mood and Depression. Trends in cognitive sciences, 13(11), 456.
Cite this article
Li,X. (2023). Identifying the relationship between depression and cognitive neuroscience. Theoretical and Natural Science,24,112-116.
Data availability
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study will be available from the authors upon reasonable request.
Disclaimer/Publisher's Note
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of EWA Publishing and/or the editor(s). EWA Publishing and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
About volume
Volume title: Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Biological Engineering and Medical Science
© 2024 by the author(s). Licensee EWA Publishing, Oxford, UK. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license. Authors who
publish this series agree to the following terms:
1. Authors retain copyright and grant the series right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgment of the work's authorship and initial publication in this
series.
2. Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the series's published
version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgment of its initial
publication in this series.
3. Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and
during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See
Open access policy for details).
References
[1]. Salehinejad, M.A., et al. (2017) Cognitive Control Dysfunction in Emotion Dysregulation and Psychopathology of Major Depression (MD): Evidence from Transcranial Brain Stimulation of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex (DLPFC). Journal of affective disorders, 210, 241.
[2]. Joormann, J. and Ema, T. (2015) Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression: Examining Cognitive Control and Emotion Regulation. Current opinion in psychology, 4, 86.
[3]. Grahek, I., et al. (2018) Cognitive Control in Depression: Toward Clinical Models Informed by Cognitive Neuroscience. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(4), 464.
[4]. Miyake, A. and Naomi, P.F. (2012) The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Current directions in psychological science : a journal of the American Psychological Society, 21(1), 8.
[5]. Koster, E.H.W., Evi de, L. and Rudi de, R. (2013) Rumination Is Characterized by Valence-Specific Impairments in Switching of Attention. Acta psychologica, 144(3), 563.
[6]. Whitmer, A.J. and Ian, H.G. (2012) Switching and Backward Inhibition in Major Depressive Disorder: The Role of Rumination. Journal of abnormal psychology, 121(3), 570.
[7]. Kaiser, R.H., et al. (2015) Distracted and down: Neural Mechanisms of Affective Interference in Subclinical Depression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(5), 654.
[8]. Auerbach, R.P., et al. (2013) Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms Underlying Cognitive Vulnerability Models of Depression. Journal of psychotherapy integration, 23(3), 222.
[9]. Bar, M. (2009) A Cognitive Neuroscience Hypothesis of Mood and Depression. Trends in cognitive sciences, 13(11), 456.